Résumés
Résumé
Le but de cet article est de discuter des facteurs de risque associés aux difficultés que les enfants d’âge scolaire peuvent éprouver dans l’apprentissage de la lecture. D’entrée de jeu, nous posons que l’instruction publique obligatoire expose les enseignants à toute la diversité des aptitudes à cet apprentissage. On estime que l’enseignement universel, en classe ordinaire, répond aux besoins d’environ 75% des apprentis lecteurs. Mais qu’en est-il des autres? Quels sont les facteurs qui, chez eux, peuvent nuire à l’apprentissage de la lecture? Pour tenter une réponse à cette question, nous faisons d’abord le portrait des principales habiletés qui président au développement des normo-lecteurs. Puis, nous distinguons deux classes de facteurs de risque : les risques individuels et les risques environnementaux. Les risques connus et centrés sur les caractéristiques de l’individu sont associés à l’hérédité, aux déficits sensoriels, aux capacités cognitives générales et aux faiblesses dans le traitement de la langue orale ou écrite. Les facteurs connus et centrés sur le milieu d’apprentissage renvoient principalement aux activités d’enrichissement ou de formation dans le foyer familial ou à l’école. Nous discutons de leur nature et de leur impact sur l’apprentissage de la lecture.
Mots-clés :
- apprentissage de la lecture,
- facteurs de risque,
- troubles de lecture
Abstract
The goal of this article is to discuss the risk factors associated with the difficulties school-age children may experience when learning to read. To begin, we suggest that with mandatory public instruction teachers must deal with the full range of students’ aptitude for learning to read. It is estimated that universal teaching, in a regular classroom, meets the needs of approximately 75% of beginning readers. But what about the others? What are the factors that, for them, may hinder reading acquisition? To address this issue, we first discuss the key skills that govern the development of normal readers. Then, we distinguish between two classes of risk factors: individual risks and environmental risks. Known individual risks are associated with heritability, sensory deficits, general cognitive abilities and oral and written language impairments. Known environmental risks refer mainly to literacy activities or instruction in the home or at school. We discuss the nature and the impact of these risk factors on reading acquisition.
Keywords:
- reading development,
- risk factors,
- reading disabilities
Veuillez télécharger l’article en PDF pour le lire.
Télécharger
Parties annexes
Bibliographie
- Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Lee, J. (2010). Kindergarten predictors of second versus eight grade reading comprehension impairments. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 332-345.
- Adnams, C. M., Sorour, P., Kalberg, W. O., Kodituwakku, P., Perold, M. D., Kotze, A., … May, P. A. (2007). Language and literacy outcome from a pilot intervention study for children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders in South Africa. Alcohol, 41, 403-414.
- Aikens, N. L., & Barbarin, O. (2008). Socioeconomic differences in reading trajectories: The contribution of family, neighbourhood, and school context. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 235-251.
- Allen, T. E. (1986). Patterns of academic achievement among hearing impaired students: 1974 and 1973. In A. A. Schildroth & M. A. Karchmer (Eds.), Deaf children in America (pp. 161-206). San Diego, CA: College Hill.
- Arriaga, R. I., Fenson, L., Cronan, T., & Pethick, S. J. (1998). Scores on the MacArthur Communication Developmental Inventory of Children from low- and middle- income families. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 209-223.
- Baddeley, A. D., & Gathercole, S. (1992). Learning to read: The role of the phonological loop. In J. Alegria, D. Holender, J. J. Morais & M. Radeau (Eds.), Analytic approaches to human cognition (pp. 153-167). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
- Badian, N. A. (2000). Do preschool orthographic skills contribute to the prediction of reading? In N. A. Badian (Ed.), Prediction and prevention of reading failure (pp. 31-56). Parkton, MD: York.
- Badian, N. A. (2001). Phonological and orthographic processing: Their roles in reading prediction. Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 179-202.
- Baker, L., Fernandez-Fein, S., Scher, D., & Williams, H. (1998). Home experiences related to the development of word recognition. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 263-287). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Bentin, S., & Leshem, H. (1993). On the interaction of psychological awareness and reading acquisition: It’s a two-way street. Annals of Dyslexia, 43, 125-148.
- Billard, C. (2007). Le dépistage des troubles du langage de l’enfant. Une contribution à la prévention de l’illettrisme. Archives pédiatriques, 8, 86-91.
- Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 1027-1050.
- Bissonnette, S., Richard, M., Gauthier, C., & Bouchard, C. (2010). Quelles sont les stratégies d’enseignement efficaces favorisant les apprentissages fondamentaux auprès des élèves de niveau élémentaire? Résultats d’une méga-analyse. Revue de recherche appliquée sur l’apprentissage, 3, 1-35.
- Boden , C., & Giaschi, D. (2007). M-stream deficits and reading-related visual processes in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 346-366.
- Bogliotti, C., Serniclaes, W., Messaoud-Galusi, S., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2008). Discrimination of speech sounds by children with dyslexia: Comparisons with chronological age and reading level. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 137-155.
- Bowey, J. A. (1995). Socioeconomic status differences in preschool phonological sensitivity and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 476-487.
- Bowey, J. A. (2005). Predicting individual differences in learning to read. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 155-172). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
- Bus, A. G., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 403-414.
- Byrne, B. (1998). The foundation of literacy: The child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
- Byrne, B., Wadsworth, S., Corley, R., Samuelson, S., Quain, P., DeFries, J. C., ... & Olson, R. K. (2005). Longitudinal twin study of early literacy development: Preschool and kindergarten phases. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 219-235.
- Castles, A., Bates, T., Coltheart, M., Luciano, M., & Martin, N. G. (2006). Cognitive modelling and the behaviour genetics of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 92-103.
- Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149-180.
- Catach, N. (2005). L’orthographe française. Paris: Armand Colin.
- Catts, H. W., Fey, E. M., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 45, 1142-1157.
- Catts, H. W., Fey, E. M., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331-361.
- Catts, H. W., Fey, E. M., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Estimating the risk of future reading failure difficulties in kindergarten children: A research-based model and its clinical implementation. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 38-50.
- Chamberlain, C., & Mayberry, R. (2000). Theorizing about the relationship between ASL and reading. In C. Chamberlain, J. Morford, & R. Mayberry (Eds.), Language acquisition by eye (pp. 221-259). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Chevrie-Muller, C. (2007). Troubles spécifiques du développement du langage. Dysphasies de développement. In C. Chevrie-Muller & J., Narbora (Eds.), Le langage de l’enfant (pp. 361-420) Paris, France: Éditions Masson.
- Chiappe, P., Chiappe, D. L., & Gottardo, A. (2004). Vocabulary, context, and speech perception among good and poor readers. Educational Psychology, 24, 825-843.
- Christin, A. M. (2001). Histoire de l’écriture: de l’idéogramme au multimédia. Paris: Flammarion.
- Colin, S., Magnan, A., Écalle, J., & Leybaert, J. (2007). Relation between deaf children’s phonological skills in kindergarten and word recognition performance in first grade. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 139-146.
- Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed), Strategies of information processing (pp. 151-216). London: Academic Press.
- Coltheart, M. (2005). Modeling reading: The dual-route approach. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 6-23). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
- Conrad, R. (1979). The deaf school child. London: Harper & Row.
- Corriveau, K. H., Goswami, U., & Thompson, J. M. (2010). Auditory processing and early literacy skills in a preschool and kindergarten population. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 369-382.
- Cowan, L., & Bobier, W. R. (2003). The pattern of astigmatism in a Canadian preschool population. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 44, 4593-4600.
- Daigle, D., & Armand, F. (2008). Phonological sensitivity in severely and profoundly deaf readers of French. Reading & Writing, 21, 699-717.
- Daigle, D., Berthiaume, R., & Demont, E. (in press). The effect of the task in deaf readers’ graphonological processes: A longitudinal study. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education.
- Day, K. C., & Day, H. C. (1984). Kindergarten knowledge of print conventions and later school achievement: A five-year follow-up. Psychology in the School, 21, 393-396.
- Desrochers, A., & Berger, M. J. (2011). Littératie, éducation et société. In M. J. Berger & A. Desrochers (Eds.), L’évaluation de la littératie (pp. 7-27). Ottawa: Presses de l’Université d’Ottawa, Canada.
- Desrochers, A., Cormier, P., & Thompson, G. (2005). Sensibilité phonologique et apprentissage de la lecture. Parole, 34-35-36, 113-138.
- Desrochers, A., Kirby, J. R., Thompson, G. L., & Fréchette, S. (2009). Le rôle de la conscience phonologique dans l’apprentissage de la lecture. Revue du Nouvel Ontario, 34, 145-169.
- Eden, G. F., Stein, J. F., Wood, M. H., & Wood, F. B. (1995). Verbal and visual problems in dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 272-290.
- Ehri, L. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In P. Gough, L. C. Ehri & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107-143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Ehri, L. C. (2005). Development of sight word reading: Phases and findings. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 135-154). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
- Elbro, C. Borstrom, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from Kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 36-60.
- Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Barnes, M. A. (2007). Learning disabilities: From identification to intervention. New York: Guilford.
- Fraser, G. R. (1974). Epidemiology of profound childhood deafness. Audiology, 13, 335-341.
- Fréchette, S., & Desrochers, A. (2011). Le dépistage des élèves à risque d’éprouver des difficultés en lecture. In M. J. Berger & A. Desrochers (Eds.), L’évaluation de la littératie (pp. 81-116). Ottawa, Canada: Presses de l’Université d’Ottawa.
- Galaburda, A. M., & Sherman, G. F. (2007). The genetics of dyslexia: What is the phenotype? In K. W. Fischer, J. H. Bernstein & M. H. Immordino-Yang (Eds.), Mind, brain, and education in reading disorders (pp. 37-58). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
- Gallagher, A., Frith, U., & Snowling, M. J. (2000). Precursors of literacy delay among children at genetic risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 203-213.
- Génard, N., Mousty, P., Content, A., Alegria, J., Leybaert, J., & Morais, J. (1998). Methods to establish subtypes of developmental dyslexia. In P. Reitsma & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Problems and intervention in literacy development (pp. 163-176). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluver.
- Gérard, C. (1993). L’enfant dysphasique. Bruxelles, Belgique: De Boeck-Wesmael.
- Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word reading and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 566-580.
- Glover, T. A., & Albers, C. A. (2007). Considerations for evaluating universal screening assessments. Journal of School Psychology, 45, 117-135.
- Gombert, J. É. (1990). Le développement métalinguistique. Paris, France: Presses universitaires de France.
- Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6-10.
- Gredler, G. R. (1997). Issues in early childhood screening and assessment. Psychology in the Schools, 34, 99-106.
- Gredler, G. R. (2000). Early childhood screening for developmental and educational problems. In B. A. Bracken (Ed.), The psychoeducational assessment of preschool children (pp. 399-411), Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
- Hammill. D. D. (1993). A brief look at the learning disabilities movement in the United States. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 295-310.
- Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1992). American parenting of language-learning children: Persisting differences in family-child interactions observed in natural home environments. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1096-1105.
- Healy, J. F. (1997). Les débuts de l’alphabet. In L. Bonfante, J. Chadwick, B. R. Cook, W. V. Davies, J. F. Healy, J. T. Hooker & C. B. F. Walker (Eds.), La naissance des écritures: du cunéiforme à l’alphabet (pp. 253-327). Paris, France: Seuil.
- Hoff, E. (2003). Causes and consequences of SES-related differences in parent-to-child speech. In M. H. Bornstein & R. H. Bradley (Eds.), Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development (pp. 147-160). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Howell, K. K., Lynch, M. E., Platzman, K. A., Smith, G. H. & Coles, C. D. (2006). Prenatal alcohol exposure, ability, academic achievement, and school functioning in adolescence: A longitudinal follow-up. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31, 116-126.
- Institut national de la santé et de la recherche médicale (2007). Dyslexie, dysorthographie, dyscalculie — Bilan des données scientifiques. Paris, France: Les Éditions Inserm.
- Jackson, N. E., & Coltheart, M. (2001). Routes to reading success and failure. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
- Katzir, T., Youngsuk, K., Wolf, M., Kennedy, B., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. (2006). The relationship of spelling recognition, RAN, and phonological awareness to reading in older poor readers and younger reading-matched controls. Reading and Writing, 19, 845-872.
- Kintsch, W., & Kintsch, E. (2005). Comprehension. In S. G. Paris & S. A. Stahl (Eds), Children’s reading comprehension and assessment (pp. 71-92). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Kirby, J. R., Desrochers, A., Roth, L., & Lai, S. S. V. (2008). Longitudinal predictors of word reading development. Canadian Psychology, 49, 103-110.
- Kirby, J. R. Geier, B. K., & Deacon, S. H. (2009). Predictors of reading: Phonological awareness, naming speed, orthographic processing, and morphological awareness. Communication présentée au congrès annuel de la Society for the Scientific Study of Reading. Boston, MA
- Kirby, J. R., Georgiou, G. K., Martinussen, R., & Parrila, R. (2010). Naming speed and reading: From prediction to instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 341-362.
- Kirby, J. R., & Savage, R. S. (2008). Can the simple view deal with the complexities of reading? Literacy, 42, 75-82.
- Kirk, S. A. (1963). Behavioral diagnosis and remediation of learning disabilities. Conference on Exploring Problems of the Perceptually Handicapped Child, 1, 1-23.
- Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150-161.
- Larrivee, L. S., & Catts, H. W. (1999). Early reading achievement in children with expressive phonological disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 118-128.
- Leseman, P. P. M., & de Jong, P. F. (1998). Home literacy: Opportunity, instruction, cooperation, and social-emotional quality predicting early reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 294-318.
- Levy, B. A., Gong, Z., Hessels, S., Evans, M. A., & Jared, D. (2006).Understanding print: Early reading development and the contributions of home literacy experiences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 63-93.
- Lewis, B. A., Freebairn, L. A., & Taylor, H. G. (2000). Follow-up of children with early expressive phonology disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 433-444.
- Leybaert, J., & Alegria, J. (1993). Is word processing involuntary in deaf children? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 1-29.
- Lindsey, K. A., Manis, F. R., & Bailey, C. E. (2003). Prediction of first-grade reading in Spanish-speaking English-language learners. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 482-494.
- Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596-613.
- Lyon, G. R. (1998). Overview of reading and literacy initiatives. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
- Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1-14.
- Malacova, E., Blair, J. L., Mattes, E., de Klerk, N., & Stanley, F. (2009). Neighbourhood socioeconomic status and maternal factors at birth as moderators of the association between birth characteristics and school attainment: A population study of children attending government schools in Western Australia. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 63, 842-849.
- Manis, F. R. Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M., McBride-Chang, C., & Peterson, A. (1996). On the basis of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157-195.
- Mayberry, R. I., del Giudice, A. A., & Lieberman, A. M. (2011). Reading achievement in relation to phonological coding and awareness in deaf readers: A meta-analysis. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16, 164-188.
- Morton, J. (2004). Understanding developmental disorders: A causal modelling approach. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
- Musselman, C. (2000). How do children who can’t hear learn to read an alphabetic script? A review of the literature on reading and deafness. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 9-31.
- Muter, V., & Diethelm, K. (2001). The contribution of phonological skills and letter knowledge to early reading development in a multilingual population. Language Learning, 51, 187-219.
- Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phoneme, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 665-681.
- Nation, K., & Cocksey, J. (2009). The relationship between knowing a word and reading it aloud in children’s word reading development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 296-308.
- National Early Literacy Panel (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy.
- Noble, K. G., Farah, M. J., & McCandliss, B. D. (2006). Socioeconomic background modulates cognitive-achievement relationships in reading. Cognitive Development, 21, 349-368.
- Nosarti, C., Walshe, M., Rushe, T. M., Rifkin, L., Wyatt, J., Murray, R. M., & Allin, M. P. (2011). Neonatal ultrasound results following very preterm birth predict adolescence behavioral and cognitive outcome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 118-135.
- OCDE & Statistique Canada (1995). Littératie, économie et société: résultats de la première Enquête internationale sur l’alphabétisation des adultes. Paris and Ottawa: Author.
- OCDE & Statistique Canada (2005). Apprentissage et réussite: premiers résultats de l’Enquête sur la littératie et les compétences des adultes. Paris and Ottawa: Author.
- Olson, R. K. (2008). Genetic and environmental influences on word-reading skills. In E. L. Grigorenko & A. J. Naples (Eds.), Single-word reading: Behavioral and biological perspectives (pp. 233-253). New York, NY: Erlbaum.
- Olson, R. K., Datta, H., Gayán, J., & DeFries, J. C. (1999). A behavioral-genetic analysis of reading disabilities and component processes. In R. M. Klein & P. A. McMullen (Eds.), Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia (pp. 133-153). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
- Pennington, B. F., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development, 72, 816-833.
- Pennington, B. F., & Olson, R. K. (2005). Genetics of dyslexia. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 453-472). Oxford, UK: Backwell.
- Phillips, B. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2005). Social correlates of emergent literacy. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 173-187). Oxford, UK: Backwell.
- Rodda, M., & Grove, C. (1987). Language, cognition, and deafness. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Rouse, M. W., Borstein, E., Hyman, I., Hussein, M., Cotter, S. A., Flynn, M., ... De Land, P. N. (1999). Frequency of convergence insufficiency among fifth and sixth graders: The convergence insufficiency and reading study (CIRS) group. Optometry and Vision Science, 76, 643-649.
- Scanlon, D. M., & Vellutino, F. R. (1997). A comparison of the instructional backgrounds and cognitive profiles of poor, average, and good readers who were initially identified as at risk for reading failure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 191-215.
- Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities. In B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Accardo, & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75-119). Timonium, MD: York Press, Inc.
- Scarborough, H. S. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading disability: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 97-110). New York, NY: Guilford.
- Scarborough, H. S., & Dobrich, W. (1990). Development of children with early language delay. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 70-83.
- Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265-282.
- Schatschneider, C., Carlson, C. D., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., & Fletcher, J. M. (2002). Relationships of rapid automatized naming and phonological awareness in early reading development: Implications for the double deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 245-256.
- Scheiman, M., Cooper, J. Mitchell, L., Deland, P., Cotter, S. Borsting, E., … Rouse, M. (2002). A survey of treatment modalities for convergence insufficiency. Optometry and Vision Science, 79, 151-157.
- Schneider, W., Roth, E., & Ennemoser, M. (2000). Training phonological skills and letter knowledge in children at risk for dyslexia: A comparison of three kindergarten intervention programs. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 284-295.
- Séguin, H., & Desrochers, A. (2008). L’évolution de l’orthographe au fil des dictionnaires de l’Académie française. In A. Desrochers, F. Martineau & Y. C. Morin (Eds.), Orthographe française: évolution et pratique (pp. 91-137). Ottawa, Canada: Éditions David.
- Sénéchal, M. (2006). Testing the home literacy model: Parent involvement in kindergarten is differentially to grade 4 reading comprehension, fluency, spelling, and reading for pleasure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 59-87.
- Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 445-460.
- Sénéchal, M., LeFevre, J., Smith-Chant, B. L., & Colton, K. V. (2001). On refining theoretical models of emergent literacy: The role of empirical evidence. Journal of School Psychology, 39, 439-460.
- Sénéchal, M., Ouellette, G., & Rodney, D. (2006). The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary to future reading. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 173-182). New York, NY: Guilford.
- Seymour, P. H. K. (1997). Foundations of orthographic development. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages (pp. 319-337). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Seymour, P. H. K. (2005). Early reading development in European orthographies. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 296-315). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
- Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151-218.
- Share, D. L. (2011). On the role of phonology in reading acquisition: The self-teaching hypothesis. In S. A. Brady, D. Braze & C. A. Fowler (Eds.), Explaining individual differences in reading (pp. 45-68). New York: Psychology Press.
- Share, D., & Stanovich, K. (1995). Cognitive processes in early reading development: Accommodating individual differences into a model of acquisition. Issues in Education: Contribution to Educational Psychology, 1, 1-57.
- Shankar, S., Evans, M. A., & Bobier, W. R. (2007). Hyperopia and the emergent literacy of young children: Pilot study. Optometry and Vision Science, 84, 1031-1038.
- Shaywitz, S. E., Escobar, M. D., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Makuch, R. (1992). Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of a normal distribution of reading ability. New England Journal of Medicine, 326, 145-150.
- Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M., Holahan, J. M., Schneider, A. E., Marchione, K. E., Stuebing, K. K., … Shaywitz, B. (1999). Persistence of dyslexia: The Connecticut longitudinal study at adolescence. Pediatrics, 104, 1351-1359.
- Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Escobar, M. D. (1990). Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls: Results of the Connecticut longitudinal study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 264, 998-1002.
- Simkin, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Evidence of reading difficulty in subgroups of children with specific language impairment, Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 22, 315-331.
- Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358-373.
- Snowling, M. J., Muter, V., & Carroll, J. (2007). Children at family risk of dyslexia: A follow-up in early adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 609-618.
- Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Kipffer-Piquard, A., Pinton, F., & Billard, C. (2009). Reliability and prevalence of an atypical development of phonological skills in French-speaking dyslexics. Reading and Writing, 22, 811-842.
- Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., & Serniclaes, W. (2006). Reading acquisition and developmental dyslexia. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
- Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Mathew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360-407.
- Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S., & Gottardo, A. (1997). Converging evidence for phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 114-127.
- Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D.V.M., Chipchase, B. B., & Kaplan, C. A. (1998). Language-impairment preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 41, 407-418.
- Stuart, M. (1995). Prediction and qualitative assessment of five and six year old children’s reading: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 287-296.
- Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second to fifth grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161-185.
- Vaessen, A., Gerretsen, P., & Blomert, L. (2009). Naming problems do not reflect a second independent core deficit in dyslexia: Double deficits explored. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 202-221.
- Valdois, S., & Bosse, M. L. (2011). Visual attention span and reading acquisition. Communication au congrès annuel de la Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, St. Pete Beach, FL.
- Van Horn Melton, J. (1988). Absolutism and the eighteenth-century origins of compulsory schooling in Prussia and Austria. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
- Vaughn, S. J., & Klingner, J. (2007). Overview of the three-tier model of reading intervention. In D. Haager, J. Klingner & S. Vaughn (Eds.), Evidence-based reading practices for response to intervention (pp. 3-9). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
- Vaughn, S. J., Wanzek, J., Woodruff, A. L., & Linan-Thompson, S. (2007). Prevention and early identification of students with reading disabilities. In D. Haager, J. Klingner & S. Vaughn (Eds), Evidence-based reading practices for response to intervention (pp. 11-27). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
- Vellutino, F. R., & Fletcher, J. M. (2005). Developmental dyslexia. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 362-378). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
- Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Lyon, G. R. (2000). Differentiating between difficulty to remediate and readily remediated poor readers: More evidence against the IQ-achievement discrepancy definition of reading disability. Journal of Learning Disability, 33, 223-238.
- Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., Sipay, E., Small, S., Pratt, A., Chen, R., & Denckla, M. (1996). Cognitive profiles of difficult-to-remediate and readily-remediated poor readers: Early intervention as a vehicle for distinguishing between cognitive and experiential deficits as basic causes of specific reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 601-638.
- Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Spearing, D. (1995). Semantic and phonological coding in poor and normal readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 76-123.
- Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., … Garon, T. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468-479.
- Walley, A. C., Metsala, J. L., & Garlock, V. M. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Reading and Writing, 16, 5-20.
- Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (2001). Emergent literacy: Development from pre-readers to readers. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickensen (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 11-29). New York: Guilford.
- Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668-680.
- Wise, J. C., Sevcik, R. A., Morris, R. D., Lovett, M. W., & Wolf, M. (2007a). The growth of phonological awareness by children with reading disabilities: A result of semantic knowledge or knowledge of grapheme-phoneme correspondences? Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 151-164.
- Wise, J. C., Sevcik, R. A., Morris, R. D., Lovett, M. W., & Wolf, M. (2007b). The relationship among receptive and expressive vocabulary, listening comprehension, pre-reading skills, word identification skills, and reading comprehension by children with reading disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50, 1093-1109.
- Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415-438.
- Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387-407.